Welcome to my facts page! My goal here is to summarize some
of the research relevant to LGBT issues. This is not a comprehensive treatment
of the science of human sexuality, but it is an attempt to highlight basic information
about a variety of LGBT topics, especially facts which challenge misinformation
about sexual minorities. I hope this compilation will be a valuable resource to
anyone interesting in knowing more about the science or sociology of sexual
orientation and gender expression. I’ve included links to some individual
research studies in the citations list at the bottom of the page, but often
you’ll need access to a research library for the full contents of any specific
article. This page is a work-in-progress (and some topics are poorly addressed), so please return at a future time for
updates and additions. I welcome suggestions about important studies that I have
not included below, so feel free to send me a message!
Version: 15 January 2018
Content
Section 1: Prevalence of sexual minorities in the human
population
Section 2: The biological origins of homosexuality
Section 3: Homosexuality in other species
Section 4: Stability and fluidity of sexual orientation
Section 5: LGBT relationships
Section 6: LGBT physical and mental health
SECTION 1. Prevalence of sexual minorities in the human
population.
1.1 What percentage of people are gay, lesbian, bisexual or
transgender?
There are different answers to this seemingly simple
question, depending on how sexuality is defined. For example, sexuality can be
defined based on attraction, based on behavior, or based on self-identity
(Bailey et al. 2016). Behavior or identity doesn’t always coincide with
romantic or sexual attraction, so some people may have had a lifetime of same-sex
attraction but no sexual experiences with the same sex. Similarly, some people
may have had same-sex sexual experiences at some point in life, but don’t chose
to identify as gay or bisexual (Bailey et al. 2016).
Alfred Kinsey published the earliest well-known studies of
the prevalence of same-sex behaviors during the 1940s-1950s. He concluded that 37%
of men had had at least one same-sex sexual encounter during their lifetime
(Sell et al. 1995). Kinsey’s numbers have been criticized because they were not
based on a representative sample of adults which would provide a more accurate
estimate of the incidence of same-sex behavior in the human population as a
whole.
Sell et al. (1995) studied the prevalence of same-sex
attraction and behavior in men and women from the US ,
the UK , and France . They found
that since age 15, between 18-21% of Americans had experienced either homosexual
behavior or attraction. They found similar percentages in Europe .
During the 5 year period just before the study, 6.2% of US male participants
and 3.3% of US female participants reported some same-sex sexual contact.
Figure 1. Percentage of sexual minority groups among the LGBT population. Data from Pew (2013). |
Those who identify
as LGBT appear to be a smaller percentage of people than those who have had
some same-sex experiences during their life. The Williams Institute (affiliated
with UCLA’s School of Law ) compiled information from four recent
nation-wide population studies of LGBT identity in the US (Gates
2014). Each of the four primary studies were conducted on thousands of people. Together
they suggest that between 2.2 and 4.0% of adult Americans identify as lesbian,
gay, bisexual, or transgender (5.2-9.5 million US adults). Similar estimates have
been obtained from non-western cultures (Bailey et al. 2016). Estimates of gay
and lesbian-identified Americans ranged from 1.4 to 1.8%, while estimates of people
who identify as bisexual were more varied among the studies, ranging from 0.6%
to 2.6%. There were no detailed figures available on the percentage of
transgender people among American adults.
The studies reviewed by Gates (2014) also revealed several
other demographic trends about LGBT people in the United States . First, more women tend
to identify as LGBT than men. Second, younger individuals are more likely to
identify as LGBT than older Americans. Finally, in most studies the racial and
ethnic makeup of the LGB population is similar proportionally to the non-LGB
population (in other words, no particular racial group is more likely to be
LGBT than others).
One factor that might make it difficult to carefully
determine the percentage of LGBT persons in the general population is the hesitancy
to self-report sexual behaviors or identity. Individuals who are in the closet
may not accurately answer survey questions about their romantic attractions or
sexual experiences. The magnitude of this phenomenon is unknown, but in-so-far
as non-disclosure affects the accuracy of survey results, the proportion of
LGBT people in a population is more likely to be underestimated than
overestimated.
In the United States, LBGT people live all over the country,
but a Gallup poll conducted in 2012 suggests that states vary in their
percentages of LGBT-identified individuals. The District
of Columbia , Hawaii , Vermont , and Oregon
topped the list with the highest percentages of LGBT people (10.0, 5.1, 4.9 and
4.9% respectively), while Tennessee , Mississippi , Montana and
North Dakota
had the lowest percentages (only 1.7 to 2.6%).
Interestingly, Gallup polling
conducted as recently as 2011 suggests that Americans vastly over-estimate the
percentage of gay people in the US
population. On average, Americans estimated that 25% of all people living in
the US
are gay. Only 4% of American adults correctly guessed that less than 5% of
Americans are LGBT.
Sources:
- Bailey et al.
2016 Psychol Sci Public Interest
- Gates 2014.
LGBT demographics
- Sell et al.
1995. Archiv Sexual Behavior
1.2 What do the letters in the acronym LGBTQIA stand for?
L = lesbian; G = gay, B = bisexual; T = transgender; Q =
queer or questioning; I = intersex; A = asexual (or it is sometimes used to
denote a straight ally)
1.3 What is bisexuality?
Human sexuality is not simply binary. It exists on a
continuum, from homosexuality to bisexuality (attraction to both sexes) to
heterosexuality. One way to depict this continuum in a semi-quantitative way is
through use of the Kinsey scale. On this scale, an exclusively heterosexual
person is a 0, an exclusively homosexual individual is a 6, and bisexually-oriented
people fall between 1 and 5. On the Kinsey scale, a person who identifies as
straight and is primarily attracted to the opposite sex, but who has occasional
same-sex attraction or behaviors might be classified as a 1 or 2.
Although the gay or lesbian community sometimes seems to be
more visible (and discussed) in American society than bisexuals, some survey
data, including the 2006-2010 National Survey of Family Growth, found that more
Americans may identify as bisexual than gay or lesbian (Gates 2014). However
among surveys, the prevalence of bisexuality appears to vary considerably.
Bisexuality exists in both men and women, it but tends to be
more prevalent for women. A compilation of recent surveys from the William
Institute suggests that 68-77% of bisexuals are female (Gates 2014). Consistent
with this finding are studies that suggest that men tend to have a more
“polarized” sexual orientation than women (Lippa 2007). In other words, they
tend to fall pretty close to either end of the Kinsey scale (Lippa 2007) with
less incidence of bisexuality (Mock & Eibach 2011). Differences in the incidence
of bisexuality among men and women may due to socialization factors as children
grow up (e.g., gender non-conforming behavior may be more accepted in girls in
some societies), or differences in hormone-induced changes to the young human
brain during development (Lippa 2007). In a worldwide study of over 200,000
people responding to a BBC survey, Lippa (2007) found consistent patterns of
sexual orientation polarization between men and women, suggesting that
biological factors lead to these differences. As addressed below, women also
tend to have greater fluidity in their sexual orientation than men.
Some have postulated that bisexuality is a transitional
period in the course of greater self acceptance that one is gay, or a period of
same-sex experimentation for people who are truly heterosexual. However,
long-term observation of bisexual/unlabeled women by Diamond (2008) shows more
support for bisexuality as a relatively stable sexual orientation.
Sources:
- Diamond 2008.
Developmental Psychol
- Gates 2014.
LGBT demographics.
- Lippa 2007.
Archiv Sexual Behavior
- Mock &
Eibach 2011. Archiv Sexual Behavior
1.4 Does sexuality encompass more than sexual behaviors?
Yes, human sexuality encompasses a range of feelings and
behaviors including sex, courtship, romance, and social status. For example, a
gay person may be likely to be strongly attracted to the same sex emotionally
as well as sexually. While the Kinsey scale measures the degree of same-sex
sexual interest, this second (emotional) dimension of sexuality can also be
represented in a conceptual framework. In 1985, Klein and co-authors introduced
the Klein Sexual Orientation Grid that incorporates multiple dimensions of
attraction and orientation. Thinking about sexuality as occurring along two,
three, or even more dimensions of human attraction, shows the diversity of
romantic, emotional, and sexual attraction manifest in human beings.
Figure 2. The Klein Sexual Orientation Grid, illustrating that sexual orientation encompasses multiple aspects of attraction. |
Source:
- Klein et al. 1985. J Homosexuality
1.5 When do people first report feelings of same-sex
attraction?
Anecdotally, many of us have heard of LGBT people who knew
they were “different” at a very young age. In a 2013 survey of about 1200 LGB
Americans, the Pew Research Center found that the median age at which LGB
Americans first thought they were not heterosexual ranged from 10 years old
(gay men) to 13 (lesbian women and bisexuals). The median age at which
non-heterosexuals first disclosed their feelings (“came out”) to other people
ranged from 18 years old in gay men, to 20 for bisexuals, and 21 for lesbians.
These data show that most LGBT people wait several years between personally
discovering that they belong to a sexual minority population and disclosing
that information to others.
Source:
- Pew Research
Center . 2013. A survey of
LGBT Americans.
1.6 What is asexuality?
A small percentage of people describe themselves as asexual.
These individuals may have little or no interest in sexual activities with
either biological sex. However, other components of human attraction, such as
romantic feelings, may be important to asexual people. Some asexual individuals
are sexually active (e.g., with a spouse) although their interest in sex may be
minimal. While public discussion of LGBT issues has exploded in recent years,
there is still very little discussion about asexuality.
Sex drive tends to be higher overall in men than in women
(Lippa 2007). Additionally, there appears to be very small differences in sex
drive between sexual orientations: in an international sample, gay and bisexual
men reported a slightly lower sex drive than straight men, and bisexual women
reported a slightly higher sex drive than straight and lesbian women. Other
data suggests that bisexuals may be more sexually active on average than
exclusively heterosexual or homosexual individuals (van Wyk & Geist 1995).
Sources:
- Lippa 2007.
Archiv Sexual Behavior
- van Wyk &
Geist 1995. J Homosexuality
1.7 What does transgender mean?
Being transgender is a fairly different phenomenon from
being gay, lesbian, bisexual or asexual. Those identities are concerned with
who a person is attracted to and how. Transgender individuals feel that they
have a different gender identity than the one they were born with, or grew up
with. Transgender people may or may not choose elective surgery or hormone
therapy so that their physical bodies more closely resemble their gender
identity. Many gay, lesbian, bisexual or asexual people feel that their gender
identity is consistent with their biological sex (they are “cis-gender”). To
put it simply, gender identity is how one feels about oneself, while sexual
orientation is how one feels about other human beings.
SECTION 2. The biological origins of homosexuality
2.1 Is human sexuality
due to biological factors or personal choice?
The exact origins of homosexuality in humans are not fully
known, nor can sexuality necessarily be attributed to a single factor for all
people. Several biological factors may play a role in the development of sexual
orientation (feelings and attractions). These all appear to happen early in
life (including at conception in the case of potential genetic effects). They
include genetic makeup, epigenetic expression of genes, hormone effects on
brain development in the fetus (Robinson & Manning 2000), and possible
maternal immune responses during pregnancy that affectsfetal brain development
(Bogeart 2006). Personal choice plays more of a role in sexual behavior and
identity, but it is unlikely to be the primary factor that influences
attractions themselves.
Sources:
- Bogaert.
2006. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
- Robinson
& Manning 2000. Evol Human Behavior
2.2 Is there a genetic basis for homosexuality?
Studies of twins provide evidence that homosexuality may have at least a partial genetic component (but see discussion of epigenetics below). Bailey & Pillard (1991) and
Kendler et al. (2000) studied the prevalence of homosexuality within families,
patterns which help shed light on whether sexuality is due to biological
factors or the environment in which children are raised. Bailey & Pillard
(1991) studied 161 homosexual and bisexual men with twin, non-twin, and
adoptive brothers to ascertain the degree of similarity in sexual orientation
among these different pairs of brothers. The researchers found a relatively
high degree of shared homosexual orientation among mono-zygotic (identical)
twins, but less concordance of sexual orientation for dizygotic (fraternal),
non-twin brothers, or adoptive brothers. Their data are consistent with a
biological origin (genetic and/or pre-natal development) of sexual orientation,
but also suggest that genetics are not solely responsible for sexual
orientation. If sexual orientation was determined exclusively by genetics, one
would expect that identical twins would either both be gay or both be straight
100% of the time of the time.
Kendler et al. (2000) also studied identical and fraternal
twin pairs along with non-twin sibling pairs across the US to assess
whether there was a correlation between sexual orientation in sibling pairs.
Across all of the individuals in the study, 2.8% were non-heterosexual. Among
all identical twins in the study, there was a 31.6% concordance of
non-heterosexual orientation within twin pairs. In other words, for these
twins, if one twin was gay or bisexual, there was a nearly 1 in 3 chance that
the other identical twin was also gay or bisexual. In fraternal twin pairs,
there was 8.3% concordance between non-heterosexual identity, a 1 in 12 chance
that both twins were gay or bisexual. Among all other pairs of siblings
(fraternal twins, and non-twin siblings), there was a 15.1% concordance of non-heterosexuality.
Like Bailey & Pillard (1991) the higher concordance of a gay or bisexual
identity in identical twins seen by Kendler et al. suggests that sexual orientation is inherited (genetically or epigenetically) to some degree.
One landmark study of genetics and sexual orientation was published
in the prestigious journal Science (Hamer et al. 1993). This study recruited 40
families with pairs of gay brothers with no gay relatives on the paternal side.
Analyzing 22 gene sections on the X chromosome (the sex chromosome men inherit
from their mothers), the authors found that 5 loci on the q28 region of the X
chromosome had statistically high rates of concordance between the pairs of gay
brothers , suggesting genetically-linked inheritance of male homosexuality
through the maternal side of their families. However, a subsequent study with a
larger sample size of gay men was not been able to corroborate the results of
the original study (Rice et al. 1999).
Modern sequencing techniques enable studies of large
sections of the DNA library present in all human cells (the “genome”). A recent
study by Sanders et al. (2017) examined a large percentage of human DNA in
hundreds of homosexual and heterosexual men to identify potential areas of the
genome that might be associated with homosexuality. They identified several sections
of DNA (on chromosomes 13 and 14) that showed strong statistical differences in
frequency of occurrence between gay and straight men. One gene they identified
is related to development of a part of the brain, the diencephalon, which is
known to differ in size according to sexual orientation. Another gene they
identified is related to thyroid function. More study is needed to explore potential genetic differences between gay and straight men, and to determine the
potential roles of genetics for determination of female sexual orientation and
bisexuality for all people.
Sources:
- Bailey &
Pillard 1991. Archives Gen Psychiatry
- Hamer et al.
1993 Science
- Kendler et
al. 2000. Am J Psychiatry
- Rice et al.
1999. Science
- Sanders et
al. 2017. Scientific Reports
2.3 Is sexual orientation determined by hormone exposure
during development?
Sex determination in humans and other animals starts at
conception when a fertilized egg becomes either genetically female (XX) or
genetically male (XY). (Note that although most individuals are XX or XY, other
variations in the sex chromosome arrangement, including some that are non-fatal, exist for other individuals). For XX fetuses, a default pathway of development
leads to female sex characteristics and a feminine brain. As an XY fetus
develops, male hormones (androgens) direct an alternative developmental pathway
towards a male reproductive system and masculinization of the brain (van Wyk
and Geist 1995). In animals, hormone intervention can alter the developmental
pathway despite genetic chromosomal makeup (van Wyk and Geist 1995).
In addition to biological sex determination, differences in
levels of exposure to sex hormones in developing fetuses appear to also have
effects on sexual orientation.
One apparent correlate of prenatal sex hormone exposure is
the length of the index finger relative to the length of the ring finger (a
value known as the 2D:4D ratio), which is a physical human trait that appears
to persist from childhood into adulthood (Robinson & Manning 2000). Lower
2D:4D values are associated with higher testosterone exposure, so men typically
have 2D:4D ratios that are lower than women. Additionally, Robinson and Manning
(2000) found that 2D:4D ratios in the left hands of gay and bisexual men were
lower than straight men. The implication of this finding is that prenatal testosterone
exposure for gay men is higher than for straight men, and may be even higher
yet for bisexual men. Williams et al. (2000) found that homosexual women had a
lower 2D:4D ratio than heterosexual women (suggesting they were more
masculinized during development), but did not find differences in finger length
ratios between gay and straight men.
Figure 4. A hormonal theory for the development of sexual orientation and its relationship to finger length in men. |
Sources:
- van Wyk &
Geist 1995. J Homosexuality
- Williams et
al. 2000. Nature
2.4 Are their other biological factors that could affect
sexual orientation?
Other developmental processes during pregnancy or early
childhood could potentially play a role. Ellis & Cole-Harding (2001)
studied thousands of pairs of mothers and their children to examine the
incidence of homosexuality and bisexuality in children in relation to the
stress levels of the mother during pregnancy. They found that there was a
somewhat higher chance that mothers stressed early in pregnancy would have a
gay son, although this correlation was not seen for daughters.
Another potential effect of the prenatal environment on
sexual orientation is manifest as the correlation observed between the number
of older brothers and the incidence of homosexuality in gay men. For example,
data from Robinson & Manning (2000), Bogaert (2006), and Vasey and
VanderLaan (2007) show that non-heterosexual men tend to have a greater number
of older brothers. This “birth order effect” does not appear to correlate with
sexual orientation in women (Bogaert & Skorska 2011).
Why would more older brothers affect younger brother
sexuality? It is hypothesized that this phenomenon arises because as a woman
has more male children, she tends to develop more antibodies in later pregnancies
that may target pathways involved in male brain development (Bogaert &
Skorska 2011). Bogaert & Skorska (2011) review findings that suggest the
birth order effect is due to a pre-natal mechanism, not social rearing
environment, because the effect is manifest even when older brothers are raised
separately from their younger siblings. Although the mechanism is speculative,
Bogaert & Skorska (2011) review some supportive evidence and also note its
similarity to Hemolytic Disease of the Newborn, a phenomenon where Rh- mothers
develop an immune response against Rh+ fetuses. Very recent research by Bogaert
et al. (2017) offers additional support for the immune response mechanism by
noting that mothers of homosexual sons have higher levels of antibodies against
the protein involved in male brain development (NLGN4Y) than mothers of
heterosexual sons or who don’t have children.
Sources:
- Bogaert.
2006. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
- Bogaert et
al. 2017. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
- Bogaert &
Skorska M. 2011. Front Neuroendocrinol
- Ellis &
Cole-Harding 2001. Physiol Behavior
- Robinson
& Manning 2000. Evol Human Behavior
- Vasey &
VanderLaan 2007. Proc Royal Soc B
2.5 What is epigenetics and what is its relationship with
homosexuality?
The field of epigenetics is fairly recent. To put it simply,
genetics deals with the set of traits (encoded in DNA sequences) that a person
inherits through their biological parents while epigenetics deals with how that
set of DNA codes is expressed by cells during the course of life. Put another
way, epigenetics is how a cell turns on or off the genes that are already
present in the genome of an individual.
In developing a hypothesis of epigenetic control of
sexuality, Rice et al. (2012) noted that other biological explanations such as
the maternal immune response hypothesis failed to account for all instances of
male homosexuality, and provided no account of the origins of female
homosexuality. Drawing on data from developmental biology, they
proposed an epigenetic model that could account for the development of
non-heterosexuality in both sexes.
Their idea centers on the phenomenon of “epimarks” which are
small changes to the external structure of DNA (not the base sequences
themselves) that affect the expression of DNA as it is transcribed in the cell.
Normally, most epimarks do not carry over from one generation to the next. In
the case of sexual orientation, the authors suggest that epimarks involved in hormone-induced
sexual differentiation (development of a male or female fetus) sometimes carry
over from one generation to the next. The epimarks in this instance may affect
one of several steps in the development of a male or female child based on the
amount of sensitivity of the child to androgen hormones, notably development of
sexual orientation in the brain. (Female development occurs for example, in XX
embryos that have low sensitivity to androgens like testosterone).
If a mother passes certain feminizing epimarks to a male
child (that are not erased around the time of conception) and these affect
sexual orientation but not gonad development, he may be more likely to become
homosexual or bisexual. Similarly, if a father passes along masculinizing
epimarks to a daughter, she may also be more likely to develop a non-heterosexual
orientation. Epimarks relevant to sexual development may be passed on in part
from parents, but can also be modified individually in offspring very early in
embryonic development.
The authors explain that this model of occasional inter-generational
transmission of epimarks could explain the seeming genetic inheritance of
homosexuality within families (i.e., Hamer et al. 1993), but the lack of 100%
congruence of homosexuality in twin studies (e.g., Kendler et al. 2000) that
would be expected if sexual orientation was exclusively genetically determined.
While it is an intriguing theory
consistent with lines of evidence from many studies, more data are needed to
confirm the scope of its applicability to determination of sexual orientation.
Sources:
- Hamer et al.
1993 Science
- Kendler et
al. 2000. Am J Psychiatry
- Rice et al.
2012. Quarterly Review Biol
SECTION 3. Homosexuality in other species
3.1 Do other species exhibit same-sex behaviors?
Bailey & Zuk (2009) and Neill (2009) reviewed examples
of same-sex behavior in other species. Examples include attempted mating with
the same sex (sexual behaviors), courtship, and pair-bonding. Same-sex behaviors
have been observed in many types of animals including other primates, whales,
cats, lions, elephants, birds, bats, reptiles, amphibians, insects, mollusks
and nematodes. In lizards, both male-male and female-female attempted mating
have been observed.
In other animal species there is generally not much evidence
for life-long associations between individuals of the same-sex. Additionally,
despite many examples of same-sex behavior, there do not tend to be many
examples of same-sex preference.
However, there are some exceptions to these general patterns including big horn
sheep and chinstrap penguins. Stable same-sex associations have also been
observed among pairs of birds that typically live together for years in
heterosexual pairings (Neill 2009). Elie et al. (2011) observed stable and
strong same-sex pairings in a song bird species after experimentally
manipulating ratios of males-to-female birds in a population.
Sources:
- Bailey &
Zuk. 2009. Trends Ecol Evol
- Elie et al
2011. Behav Ecol Sociobiol
- Neill J.
2009. The origins and role of same-sex relations in human societies.
3.2 Are same-sex behaviors adaptive in humans or other
animals?
Because same-sex behaviors (such as attempted mating) cannot
lead to successful reproduction, it is easy to assume that these behaviors are
maladaptive because they do not lead to successful transmission of genes across
generations. However, the prevalence of same-sex behaviors across many different
types of animal species, and the fact that they persist over evolutionary time,
suggests that these behaviors may present some adaptive advantage (or at least be
neutral with respect to organism fecundity). Bailey & Zuk (2009) explore
some of the evolutionary explanations for the occurrence of same-sex behavior
in nature. There are at least three hypotheses about how same-sex behavior may
be adaptive. First, it could aid in the formation and maintenance of social
groups that are present in some species (example: bottlenose dolphins). Second,
same-sex behaviors may increase or decrease intra-sexual conflict among
individuals (e.g., bonobos). Third, in young animals, same-sex behaviors may simply
be “practice” for successful heterosexual reproduction.
One interesting example of the potential adaptive outcome of
same-sex associations in animals comes from gulls in South
America . During El Niño years when coastal waters are warmer than
usual and there is less fish available for forage, the incidence of
female-female pairs of birds increases in the population (Neill 2009). By
rejecting males, some female birds help raise the broods of young birds born of
other females, thus increasing the chance of survival of the young birds during
climate conditions when food supplies are scarce.
In humans, Robinson & Williams (2000) speculate that
high testosterone in men (which is correlated with a greater chance of homosexuality
or bisexuality), is important for male fitness since it ensures proper
cardiovascular and urogenital development in the body. Thus, high testosterone concentration
may have a physiologically adaptive value even though it is also coupled with a
greater likelihood of homosexual or bisexual orientation.
Sources:
- Bailey &
Zuk. 2009. Trends Ecol Evol
- Neill. 2009.
The origins and role of same-sex relations in human societies.
- Robinson and
Williams 2000. Evol Human Behavior
SECTION 4. Stability and fluidity of sexual orientation
4.1 Is sexuality fluid?
Sexual orientation can change over time for some
individuals. However, most people do not seem to experience large changes in
sexual orientation over long periods of time (Kinnish et al. 2005, Mock &
Eibach 2011). Rosario et al. (2006) investigated the frequency at which LGB
youth aged 14-21 changed, or didn’t change, their sexual orientation identity
over the course of a year. They found that most gay-identified and half of
bisexual-identified youth retained the same identity over that period. They
also found that nearly half of bisexual-identified youth later identified as
gay, and that a small percentage of LGB-identified youth later identified as
straight.
The degree of change in sexual preference tends to differ
between men and women. Several studies suggest that women who identify as
lesbian or bisexual may be more likely to change their identity over time (Mock
& Eibach 2011). In contrast, a heterosexual identity was relatively stable
for both men and women in this study. Diamond (2008) studied sexual orientation
in 79 lesbian, bisexual and unlabeled young adult women over a ten year period and
found that two thirds of the study participants changed their sexual
orientation at least once during the decade. Interestingly, many of the women
with bisexual and unlabeled identities simply switched between those two
groups; lesbians were less likely to switch identities. Diamond (2008) also
found that some lesbians in her study had had some sexual contact with men,
suggesting that there may not be strict differences in behavior between lesbian
and bisexual women, even if there are differences in self-identity. In contrast
to the general assumption that women have greater sexual fluidity, Rosario et
al. (2006) found that among young LGB people who changed their identity over
the course of a year, males were more likely to change than females.
Sources:
- Diamond 2008.
Developmental Psychol
- Kinnish et
al. 2005. Archiv Sexual Behavior
- Mock &
Eibach 2011. Archiv Sexual Behavior
- Rosario et al. 2006. J Sex
Research
4.2 Is it possible to change sexual orientation?
As demonstrated in several studies, sexuality has some
fluidity, such as in youth who are developing a sexual orientation identity
(e.g., Rosario et al. 2006). However, for many people, sexuality cannot be
changed by seeking out therapy specifically for that purpose. Proactive efforts
to change sexuality through counseling, religious practices, or other means are
known as “sexual orientation change efforts” (SOCE). There was a prominent and
controversial study suggesting that sexual change orientation can be changed
(Spitzer 2003). However, Spitzer’s study has been criticized based on its
methodology and the author retracted his study about a decade after its
publication. While some individuals may experience some change in same-sex
feelings (naturally), the data suggest that most LGB people remain
non-heterosexual.
Sources:
- Spitzer 2003.
Archiv Sexual Behavior
- Rosario et al. 2006. J Sex
Research
SECTION 5. LGBT relationships
5.1 How many people are in same-sex relationships?
A national survey in the US
conducted in 2013 suggested that about 700,000 people in the US were in same-sex relationships (Gary 2014b). About 40% of
LGB people were in committed relationships or co-habiting with a partner
(either same or opposite sex), with rates of relationships tending to be higher
for women than men. About 1 in 5 LGB couples or single individuals were raising
one or more minor children at home.
Source:
Gates 2014b.
LGB Families and Relationships: Analyses of the 2013 National Health Interview
Survey
5.2 Are gay relationships healthy?
They certainly can be. Peplau et al. (1996) provided some
early discussion and statistics about same-sex relationships. Although
heterosexuals sometimes believe that same-sex relationships are less fulfilling
than straight relationships, there is no evidence that they are less so on
average than heterosexual unions. One study from the early 1980s examined the
separation rate (over an 18 month period) of couples that had been together for
at least 10 years. Separation rates were 4% for straight couples, 4% for gay
couples, and 6% for lesbian couples.
Interestingly, the stability of shorter-term relationships
(couples together for ≤ 2 yrs) appeared to be affected by the presence or
absence of societal sanction of those relationships. Among these shorter-term
couples, the separation rate was 4% for married couples, 16% for gay couples,
22% for lesbian couples, and 17% for non-married heterosexual couples.
Source:
Peplau et al.
1996. Gay and lesbian relationships. Reprinted in Kimmel and Plante.
Sexualities, 2004.
5.3 Which factors improve same-sex relationship quality?
Many of the same factors that promote happiness in
heterosexual relationships are expected to be important in same-sex
relationships. However, because of some unique aspects of same-sex
relationships (contending with homophobia, less traditionally-defined gender
roles), there may be unique considerations for gay couples. Clausell and
Roisman (2009) studied 30 gay and 30 lesbian couples and concluded that being
more open (more “out”) about one’s sexuality equated to better contentment in
the relationship.
Source:
Clausell and
Roisman. 2009. J Social Personal Relationships
5.4 What is a mixed orientation marriage?
A mixed orientation marriage (MOM) is a marriage between a
gay or bisexual person and a heterosexual person. The exact number of mixed
orientation marriages in the US
is unknown, but is estimated to be as high as 2 million (Kays and Yarhouse
2010).
Higgins (2004) studied why some gay or bisexual men might be
more likely to enter into mixed orientation marriages. He found support for the
hypothesis that gay and bisexual men that had previously married women tended
to come from families with somewhat more fundamentalist religious backgrounds,
but he didn’t find support for other hypotheses explaining the differences in
these two groups of non-heterosexual men such as differences in sexual
orientation (e.g., married men were more likely to be bisexual), or differences
in internalized homophobia. He concluded that the reasons why gay and bisexual
men marry women are poorly understood. Kays
and Yarhouse (2010) note that some LGBT people report that their sexuality was
actually a reason to get married – by marrying an opposite sex partner, they
thought they might overcome same-sex attractions. Others marry to meet the
expectations of their families or society, or because they wanted a family, or
because of love and affection for their opposite-sex partner.
Sources:
Higgins. 2004. J. Homosexuality
Kays & Yarhouse 2010. Amer J. Family Therapy
5.5 How often do mixed orientation marriages end in divorce?
What factors help them succeed?
About two thirds of mixed orientation marriages are expected
to end in divorce after the non-heterosexual spouse comes out (Kays and
Yarhouse 2010). After disclosure, the straight spouse in the relationship often
has to go through several stages of adjustment to the news, including acceptance,
reevaluation of expectations, and “moving on”, a process that can typically
take up to three years. Notably, the gay or bisexual spouse has also typically
gone through a similar process prior to the straight spouse.
Several factors appear to help improve the longevity and
quality of mixed orientation marriages. First, if the same-sex attracted
individual in the marriage is bisexual, the marriage is more likely to last.
Men or women who are higher on the Kinsey scale tend to have larger divorce rates.
Second, continued mixed orientation marriage success often requires
relationship flexibility such as an agreement that one or both partners will
have additional relationships outside the marriage (a phenomenon often called
“open marriage”; Ross 1983). Other factors like honest communication in the
relationship, a strong emotional bond between marriage partners, support from
others, and flexibility in redefining expectations within the relationship can
help mixed orientation marriages stay together (Kays & Yarhouse 2010).
Sources:
Kays & Yarhouse 2010. Amer J Family Therapy
Ross. 1983. The
married homosexual man. Routledge & Kegan Paul, London .
SECTION 6. LGBT physical and mental health
6.1 How common is suicide ideation and attempted suicide in
the LGBT community? How does this compare with the general population?
Ryan et al. (2010) found that rates of suicidal thought and
suicide attempts doubled among LGBT youth from less accepting families than
from more accepting families.
Source:
- Ryan et al.
2010. J Child Adolescent Psychiatric Nursing
6.2 How many LGBT youth are homeless?
Although nationally representative data on LGBT homelessness
may be lacking, some survey work can help provide estimates. Durso and Gates
(2012) conducted a survey of over 200 US organizations providing services
to homeless and at-risk youth (straight and LGBT). They found that
approximately 40% of the youth served by these organizations were LGBT. The National Alliance to End Homelessness estimates that there are perhaps 80,000
homeless LGBT youth nationwide, comprising 20% of homeless minors (NAEH 2012).
Sources:
- Durso
and Gates. 2012. The Williams Institute.
- National Alliance to End
Homelessness 2012. LGBTQ National Youth Policy Statement.
6.3 What are some things that parents and allies can do to
support LGBT youth?
One of the keys to self-esteem for LGBT youth is “social
connectedness”, which can be achieved for example, through youth participation
in gay-centered groups (Detrie & Lease 2007). Another important factor for
psychological well-being identified by Detrie and Lease was “perceived support”
from family and friends. Ryan et al. (2010) found that family acceptance (parents
showing accepting behaviors towards youth about their sexual identity) was
positively correlated with greater youth self esteem and general health and was
associated with less depression, substance abuse, and suicide attempts. In
contrast, a large majority (68%) of LGBT youth who were homeless or at risk of
homelessness had a history of rejection from their families, with more than
half of these youth citing abuse at home (Durso & Gates 2012).
Figure 5. Percentage of youth who believe that they can be happy as an LGBT adult according to the level of parental support they receive. Data from Ryan (2009). |
The Family Acceptance Project at San Francisco State
University has compiled
important information about how family members can support young gay, lesbian,
bisexual, and transgender individuals. For example, in a publication titled
“Supportive Families, Healthy Children”, Ryan (2009) notes that some of the ways
parents sometimes think they are helping LGBT youth such as shielding them from
LGBT friends or resources or pressuring them to be more gender conforming may
actually be harmful. In contrast, supportive behaviors from parents including
extending affection to LGBT youth, helping them connect with LGBT events and
organizations, having open discussions with their children about their LGBT
identity, and standing up for the youth to other family members who may be less
accepting.
Parental support is also correlated with the overall mental
health of LGB young adults too (Needham
& Austin 2010). LGB young men and women tend to receive lower levels of
parental support than their heterosexual peers, which is associated with a
number of negative health outcomes including a greater incidence of suicidal
thoughts, depression, and drug use. Continued parental support is thus
important to LGB individuals as they enter young adulthood.
Sources:
- Detrie &
Lease. 2007. J Homosexuality
- Durso & Gates. 2012. The Williams Institute.
- Needham & Austin 2010.
J Youth Adolescence
- Ryan 2009. Supportive
families, healthy children. Family Acceptance Project.
- Ryan et al.
2010. J Child Adolescent Psychiatric Nursing
Complete citation
list
Bailey JM, Pillard RD. 1991. A genetic study of male
sexual orientation. Archives Gen Psychiatry 48:1089-1096.
Bailey NW and Zuk M. 2009.
Same-sex sexual behavior and evolution. Trends in Ecology and Evolution
24:439-446.
Bailey JM, Vasey PL, Diamond
LM, Breedlove SM, Vilain E, Epprecht M. 2016. Sexual orientation, controversy,
and science. Psychological Science in the Public Interest 17:45-101.
Bogaert AF. 2006. Biological
versus nonbiological older brothers and men’s sexual orientation. Proceedings
of the National Academy
of Sciences of the USA
103: 10771-10774.
Bogaert AF, Skorska M. 2011.
Sexual orientation, fraternal birth order, and the maternal immune hypothesis:
a review. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 32:247-254.
Bogaert AF, Skorska MN, Wang
C, Gabrie J, MacNeil AJ, Hoffarth MR, VanderLaan DP, Zucker KJ, Blanchard R.
2017. Male homosexuality and maternal immune responsivity to the y-linked
protein NLGN4Y. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA.
Clausell E and Roisman GI.
2009. Outness, big five personality traits, and same-sex relationship quality.
Journal of Social Personal Relationships 26:211-226.
Detrie PM and Lease SM. 2007.
The relation of social support, connectedness, and collective self-esteem to
the psychological well-being of lesbian, gay and bisexual youth. Journal of
Homosexuality 53:173-199.
Diamond LM. 2008.
Female bisexuality from adolescence to adulthood: results from a 10-year
longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology 44:5-14.
Durso LE, Gates GJ. 2012.
Serving out youth: findings from a national survey of service providers working
with lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender youth who are homeless or at risk
of becoming homeless. Los Angeles :
The Williams Institute with True Colors Fund and the Palette Fund.
Elie JE, Mathevon N, Vignal
C. 2011. Same-sex pair-bonds are equivalent to male-female bonds in a life-long
socially monogamous songbird. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 65:2197-2208.
Ellis L, Cole-Harding S.
2001. The effects of prenatal stress, and of prenatal alcohol and nicotine
exposure, on human sexual orientation. Physiology and Behavior 74:213-226.
Gates GJ. 2014. LGBT
demographics: comparisons among population-based surveys. The Williams Institute, UCLA School of Law.
At:
Gates GJ. 2014b. LGB Families and Relationships: Analyses of the 2013 National Health Interview
Survey. The Williams Institute, UCLA School of Law.
Hamer DH, Hu S,
Magnuson VL, Hu N, Pattatucci AML. 1993. A linkage between DNA markers on the C
chromosome and male sexual orientation. Science 261:321-327.
Higgins DJ. 2004. Differences between previously married and never married
’gay’ men: family background, childhood experiences and current attitudes.
Journal of Homosexuality 48:19-41.
Kays JL, Yarhouse MA. 2010. Resilient factors in mixed orienttion couples:
current state of the research. The American Journal of Family Therapy
38:334-343.
Klein F, Sepekoff B, Wolf TJ.1985. Sexual
orientation: a multi-variable dynamic process. J Homosexuality 11:35-49.
Kinnish KK et al. 2005. Sex
differences in the flexibility of sexual orientation: a multi-dimensional
retrospective assessment. Archives Sexual Behavior 34:173-183.
Lippa RA. 2007. The relation
between sex drive and sexual attraction to men and women: a cross-national
study of heterosexual, bisexual, and homosexual men and women. Archives of
Sexual Behavior 36:209-222.
Mock SE, Eibach RP. 2011.
Stability and change in sexual orientation identity over a 10-year period in adulthood.
Archives of Sexual Behavior 41:641-648.
National Alliance to End
Homelessness 2012. LGBTQ National Youth Policy Statement. Federal Policy Brief,
April 19, 2012.
Neill J. 2009.
The origins and role of same-sex relations in human societies. McFarland &
Co, Inc. 470 pp.
Peplau LA,
Veniegas RC, Campbell SM. 1996. Gay and lesbian relationships. Reprinted in
Kimmel MS, Plante RF. Sexualities, 2004.
Rice G, Anderson
G, Risch N, Ebers G. 1999. Male homosexuality: absence of linkage to
microsatellite markers at Xq28. Science 284:665-667.
Rice WR, Friberg
U, Gavrilets S. 2012. Homosexuality as a consequence of epigenetically
canalized sexual development. The Quarterly Review of Biology 87:343-368.
Rosario M,
Schrimshaw EW, Hunter J, Braun L. 2006. Sexual identity development among
lesbian, gay, and bisexual youths: consistency and change over time. Journal of
Sex Research 43:46-58.
Ross MW. 1983.
The married homosexual man. Routledge & Kegan Paul, London . 184 pp.
Ryan 2009.
Supportive families, healthy children: Helping families with lesbian, gay,
bisexual & transgender children. San Francisco ,
CA : Family Acceptance Project, Marina Wright
Edelman Institute, San Francisco
State University .
Ryan C, Russel
ST, Huebner D, Diaz R, Sanchez J. 2010. Family acceptance in adolescence and
the health of LGBT young adults. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatric
Nursing 23:205-213.
Sanders AR, et
al. 2017. Genome-wide association study of male sexual orientation. Scientific Reports 7:16950.
Sell RL, Wells JA, Wypig D.
1995. The prevalence of homosexual behavior and attraction in the United States , the United
Kingdom and France : results of national
population-based samples. Archives of Sexual Behavior 24:235.
Spitzer RL. 2003. Can some
gay men and lesbians change their sexual orientation? 200 participants
reporting a change from homosexual to heterosexual orientation. Archives of
Sexual Behavior 32:403-417.
van Wyk PH, Geist GS. 1995.
Biology of bisexuality: critique and observations. Journal of Homosexuality
28:357-373.
Vasey PL, VanderLaan DP. 2007.
Birth order and male androphilia in Samoan fa’afafine. Proceedings Royal Soc B
274:1437-1442.
Williams TJ, Pepitone ME,
Christensen SE, Cooke BM, Huberman AD, Breedlove NJ, Breedlove TJ, Jordan CL,
Breedlove SM. 2000. Finger-length ratios and sexual orientation. Nature
404:455-456.
No comments:
Post a Comment